Theory of mind in older adults. The role of social and cognitive activity.

Main Article Content

Noelia López-Montilla
Carmen Barajas
Adriana Nielsen

Abstract

Theory of Mind (ToM) is defined as the ability to attribute mental states in order to understand and predict the behaviour of others. Studies on ToM in older adults show conflicting data (whether it remains stable, continues to evolve or experiences a decline) because the cognitive abilities underlying the different components of ToM (Implicit ToM, Explicit ToM) evolve differently with age and experience. This study aims to examine the role of cognitive reserve and current social and cognitive activity in age-associated changes in implicit ToM. A sample of healthy older adults (N= 157) aged 58-87 years from the province of Malaga was examined. The instruments used for the assessment were the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test and a social and cognitive activity questionnaire. The results indicate that implicit ToM decreases with age and that it is directly related to the implicit ToM ability, the experience of social and intellectual interaction throughout life and the socio-cognitive activity that the elderly maintain in the present. These findings are of relevance for the development of intervention programmes for the promotion of social cognition in older adults and the improvement of general psychological well-being.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Article Details

How to Cite
López-Montilla, N., Barajas, C., & Nielsen, A. (2021). Theory of mind in older adults. The role of social and cognitive activity. International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology. Revista INFAD De Psicología., 2(2), 351–360. https://doi.org/10.17060/ijodaep.2021.n2.v2.2242
Section
Articles

References

Bailey, P.E., Henriy, J.D., y von Hippel, W. (2008). Empathy and social functioning in late adulthood. Aging Mental Health, 12, 499-503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607860802224243

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M, Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioural and Intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113-125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1986.tb01003.x

Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C. y Robertson, M., (1997). Another Advanced Test of Theory of Mind: Evidence from Very High Functioning Adults with Autism or Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(7), 813-822. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright. S., Hill, J., Raste, Y. y Plumb, I. (2001). The ‘‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’’ Test Revised Version: A Study with Normal Adults, and Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High-functioning Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251. https://doi.org/0021-9630/01$15.00+00

Cabinio, M., Rossetto, F., Blasi, Vl, Savazzi, F., Castilli, I., Massaro, D., Valle., A., Nemni, R., Clerici, M., Marchetti, A., y Baglio, F. (2015). Mind-Reading Ability and Structural Connectivity Changes in Aging. Frontiers in Psichology, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01808

Cavallini, E., Lecce, S., Bottiroli, S., Palladino, P. y Pagnin, A. (2013). Beyond false belief: theory of mind in young, young-old, and old-old adults. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 76(3), 181-198. http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/AG.76.3.a

Duval, C., Piolino, P., Bejanin, A., Eustache, F. y Desgranges, B. (2011) Age effects on different components of theory of mind, Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 627-642. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.025

Happé, F., Winner, E., y Brownell, H. (1998). The getting of wisdom: Theory of mind in old age. Developmental Psychology, 34(2), 358-362. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.34.2.358

Kemp, J., Desprès, O., Sellal, F., y Dufour, A. (2012) Theory of Mind in normal ageing and neurodegenerative pathologies. Ageing Research Reviews, 11(2), 199-219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2011.12.001

Li, X., Wang, K., Wang, F., Tao, Q., Xie, Y. y Cheng, Q. (2013) Aging of theory of mind: The influence of educational level and cognitive processing. International Journal of Psychology, 48(4), 715-727. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2012.673724

Low, J., Apperly, I.A., Butterfill, S.A., y Rakoczy, H. (2016). Cognitive Architecture of Belief Reasoning in Children and Adults: A Primer on the Two-Systems Account. Child Development Perspectives, 10(3), 184–189. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12183

Pearlman-Avnion, S., Ron, N. y Ezekiel, S. (2018). Ageing and theory of mind abilities: The benefits of social interaction. Educational Gerontology, 44(5-6), 368–377. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601277.2018.1480130

Pezzutti, L., Longobardi, E., Milletti, K., y Ovidi, A. (2011) A study about Theory of Mind in primary and secondary aging. Life Span and Disability 14(1), 31-34. http://doi.org/2011-16541-002

Premack, D., y Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpazee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515-256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00076512

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448–460. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617702813248

Sullivan, S. y Ruffman, T. (2004). Social understanding: How does it fare with advancing years? British Journal of Psychology. 95(1), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712604322779424

Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Pérez-Sayes, G., Erekatxo-Bilbao, M. y Pelegrín-Valero, C. (2007). ¿Qué es la teoría de la mente? Revista de Neurología, 44(8): 479-489. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.4408.2006295

Westby, C. y Robinson, L. (2014). A developmental perspective for promoting Theory of Mind. Top Lang Disorders, 34(4), 362-382. https://doi.org/10.1097/TLD.0000000000000035