Estimation in the numerical line and written and mental calculation in pupils of 4th and 5th of primary education

Main Article Content

Mª Milagrosa Domínguez Suraña
Manuel Aguilar Villagrán

Abstract

Currently, the nature and development of the mental number line of children is the subject of a broad line of research. In a task on the number line, children are asked to point to a number presented on a physical number line with fixed ends. In general, it is observed that the estimates of inexperienced children and young people respond more to a logarithmic function, while the older and more experienced children reflect a linear function better. The studies on numerical line estimation and performance prediction in mathematics find strong correlation between the scores of precision in the estimation and the performance in standardized tests of mathematics (Siegler and Opfer, 2003; Siegler and Booth; 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2008; Reinert et al., 2014). Theoretical analyzes of the development of linear numerical representations suggest that board games with linear numerical lines improve the numerical knowledge of young children (Ramani and Siegler, 2008). In our context, estimation studies have been carried out in the numerical line and calculation fluency (Araujo et al., 2012) in children of the first year of Primary Education and here we approach these relationships with students of the second and third cycle of Primary Education. We have evaluated in a sample of 165 children of 4th and 5th year of Primary Education the relationship between estimation in the number line (22 numerical items in a line between 0 and 1000) and written and mental calculation (20 operations of addition, subtraction, multiplication and division that can be solved by applying the traditional calculation algorithm or mental calculation strategies). The results show the appearance of a clear linear function of the estimation in the numerical line and a negative and significant correlation between the errors of estimation and the written calculation and especially mental calculation, corroborating the findings of similar studies. The discussion focuses on the importance of constructing a number line to represent numbers, which could have a crucial role for mathematical development not only at early ages but also at older ages.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Article Details

How to Cite
Domínguez Suraña, M. M., & Aguilar Villagrán, M. (2014). Estimation in the numerical line and written and mental calculation in pupils of 4th and 5th of primary education. International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology. Revista INFAD De Psicología., 7(1), 453–462. https://doi.org/10.17060/ijodaep.2014.n1.v7.815
Section
Articles

References

Bell, N. & Tuley, K. (2003). Imagery: The sensory-cognitive connection for math. [en línea, consultado el 30 abril 2009] http://www.ldonline.org/article/5647

Bobis, J. (2007). The empty number line: a useful tool or just another procedure? Teaching Children Mathematics, 13(8), 410-413.

Bobis, J. (2008). Early spatial thinking and the development of number sense. Australian Primary Mathematics Classroom, 13, 4–9.

Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Developmental Psychology,41, 189–201.

Bruno, A. & Cabrera, N. (2005). Una mirada a la recta numérica en los libros de texto de tres editoriales españolas. Formación del Profesorado e Investigación en Educación Matemática, 7, 33-56.

Bruno, A. & Cabrera, N. (2006). Types of representations of the number line text books. Proceedings of the Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education. Volume 2. Prague: Czech Republic.

Clarke, D. M., Downton, A. & Roche, A. (2011). Learn a simple game to support your students’ mathematical learning across a range of content areas. Teaching Children Mathematics, 17(6), 342-349.

de Hevia, M.D., & Spelke, E.S. (2010). Number-space mapping in human infants. Psychological Science, 21, 653-660.

Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense. New York: Oxford University Press.

Dehaene, S. (2001). Precis of “the number sense”. Mind and Language, 16, 16–32.

Diezmann, C. M. & Lowrie, T. (2006). Primary Students’ Knowledge of and Errors on Number Lines. In Identities, Cultures, and Learning Spaces: Proceedings of the 29th Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia (MERGA), pp. 171–78. Adelaide, Australia: Merga.

Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Byrd-Craven, J. (2008). Development of number line representations in children with mathematical learning disability. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 277-299.

Gervasoni, A. (2005). Opening Doors to Successful Learning for Those Who Are Vulnerable.” In Judy Mousley, Leicha Bragg, & Coral Campbell (Eds). Mathematics:Celebrating Achievement: Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Conference of the Mathematics Association of Victoria (MAV), (pp. 125–36). Brunswick, Victoria: MAV.

Griffin, S., Case, R., & Siegler, R. S. (1994). Rightstart: Providing the central conceptual prerequisites for first formal learning of arithmetic to students at risk for school failure. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice (pp. 25-49). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Griffin, S. A. & Case, R. (1996). Evaluating the breadth and depth of training effects when central conceptual structures are taught. En R. Case & Okamoto (Eds.). The role of central structures in the development of children’s thought (pp. 83-102). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61, serial, 246, 1-2.

Ineson, E. (2008). The mental mathematics of trainee teachers in the UK: Patterns and preferences. The mathematics educator, 11, 1/2, 127-142.

LeFevre, J. A., Greenham, S. L., & Waheed, N. (1993). The development of procedural and conceptual knowledge in computational estimation. Cognition and Instruction, 11, 95–132.

Newcombe, N. S. (2002). The nativist-empiricist controversy in the context of recent research on spatial and quantitative development. Psychological Science, 13, 395–401.

Opfer, J., & Siegler, R. S. (2007). Representational change and children’s numerical estimation. Cognitive Psychology, 55, 169– 195.

Petitto, A. L. (1990). Development of numberline and measurement concepts. Cognition and Instruction, 7, 55–78.

Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children’s numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child Development, 79, 375– 394.

Ramani, G.B., Siegler, R.S. & Hitti, A. (2012). Taking it to the classroom: Number board games as a small group learning activity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104 (3), 661-672.

Reinert, R.M., Huber, S., Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K. (2014). Multiplication facts and the mental number line: evidence from unbounded number line estimation (2014) Psychological Research, pp. 1-9. (in press).

Schneider, M., Grabner, R. H., & Paetsch, J. (2009). Mental number line, number line estimation, and mathematical achievement: Their interrelations in grades 5 and 6. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 359-372. doi:10.1037/a0013840

Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Development, 75, 428–444.

Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2005). Development of numerical estimation: A review. In j. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 197212) New York: Psychology Press.

Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science, 14, 237–243.

Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2008). Playing board games promotes low-income children’s numerical development. Developmental Science, 11, 655–661.